According to the primatologist Frans de Waal, humankind is
suffering from “anthropodenialism”
– a refusal to accept the complexity of other animals. Because, he says, if we reject
the notion that animals can feel pain, possess a consciousness, a mental life
or self-awareness – that they are in many ways similar to us (or we are similar
to them) – then any amount of cruelty or instrumental treatment is justified. I
remember reading about vivisectionist monks in the 17th century, who
following the philosopher René Descartes, were convinced that
animals were merely machines so when they strapped dogs to tables and cut them
open the resulting screams were, in their view, just due to air coming out.
This is where the humanistic arrogance that humans are
somehow unique seems to lead.
There has been progress since then but it’s been slow, attritional
and selective. In March last year, for example, Switzerland became one of the
first countries to make it illegal
to boil lobsters without stunning them first. Italy had already made the
practice illegal and in 2017 an Italian court ruled that freezing lobsters in
ice before cooking them was cruel. In 2008 Switzerland, which seems to be a
pioneer in animal welfare, made illegal to own a single guinea pig because
it will get lonely.
The reason why headway is so fitful, I think, is fear of
where giving ground on this issue will ultimately end. If they are sentient and
feel pain, is it ever right kill animals for food? Or should we grade them and
exempt those with ‘higher consciousness’ like octopuses and squid?
Hiding from ourselves
However, there is also an opposite danger – though one that
is rarely voiced. That is in asserting that there is no human-animal duality,
but rather a continuum, we lose sight of how humans are different – as a result
of evolution – from every other animal species on earth. And how, in indulging
in such denial, we evade responsibility for dealing with our own creativity and
intelligence.
In fact, de Waal illustrates the danger. On one level, it’s
hard to disagree with him that insisting on human uniqueness has justified –
and still does legitimise – cruel behaviour that, in reality, has no
justification. But de Waal does not leave it there. Since our emotions are no
different from the emotions of most mammals, and since like most primates, “we
are a hierarchical species”, the putatively civilised institutions we spend
our lives within bear the unconsciousness imprint of our animalistic nature. They
are, in fact, eerily similar to the social structures of chimpanzees, our
closest animal relative (we share 98% of our DNA). In denying this affinity, we
are hiding from ourselves.
The English writer Will Storr, clearly influenced by de
Waal, elaborates. “By observing behaviours the human self shares with the chimp
self,” he writes in Selfie: How the West Became Self-Obsessed,
“we might find clues to which parts of us are so old they predate our ascent to
the top of the world.”
These mutual behaviours include hiding feelings to get your
way, bearing long-term grudges and manipulating others. Above all, there is a
common preoccupation with hierarchy – younger chimpanzees engage in
conspiracies and coups to topple the ruling alpha male, group members form
shifting alliances and engage in ‘political’ beatings and murders. And once in
power, the leader doesn’t just need prove his physical prowess; he needs to be
an arbiter of disputes, to disrupt bonding amongst rivals and act as a
protector of low ranked chimps.
“… despite the din and wizardry of modern life,” writes
Storr, “despite how separate we feel from the beasts, the truth is that we are
great apes that sit in the primate superfamily Hominoidea. We are modern yet
ancient, advanced yet primitive. We are animals.”
The other side of the family
But if the chimp-human analogy is a little too right-wing
for your taste – all that hierarchy, dominant alpha males and violence – there
is an alternative. Step forward the bonobo, the so-called ‘caring, sharing’ ape
that we are equally closely related to (we share 98% of our DNA with both
chimpanzees and bonobos). Unlike with chimps, female bonobos are at least as
important as males and dominance hierarchies are much less pronounced. Bonobos, who live the Congo
basin and are classed as an endangered species, also
share food with strangers, express empathy, hug, kiss, and use sex, rather
than violence, as a means of resolving disputes (male bonobos, apparently, get
erections at the prospect of food).
A group of Bonobo apes
So, we have a choice it seems – we can prefer to believe we
are more like chimps or more in tune with bonobos. There is no correct answer –
why de Waal, who has written books about both chimpanzees and bonobos, feels we
have more in common with chimps is not immediately obvious.
But what if the answer to the puzzle, ‘Are we more like
chimps or bonobos?’ is neither?
This is not to deny that we ‘possess’ the suite of
behaviours on show here. Clearly, humans can be aggressive, manipulative,
deceitful, form informal alliances with others and jockey for power. They can
also be empathetic, like the bonobo. Richard Wilkinson and Kate Pickett in The Spirit Level say that we possess “mirror neurons” in our brain that enable us
to make the same movements as others in sympathy. So, for example, people
routinely flinch when they see someone experience pain in a film they are
watching.
Open to subversion and change
However, crucially, our social arrangements do not simply stem
from the way we behave towards each other. This can be seen in the hunter-gatherer
tribes in which homo sapiens have spent 9/10ths of their time on
earth, mostly dissipating from around 10,000 years ago. These were comparable
in scope to chimp and bonobo bands. But they were qualitatively different and
not just in terms of intelligence and the use of technology. Early humanity –
in contrast to other apes or any other animal species – was quite aware of the
danger of dominant individuals seizing power and took steps to ensure that the
possibility of egalitarianism was preserved and that no social or political
order became eternally fixed.
It’s actually not straightforward to establish how our
hunter-gatherer ancestors lived. The assumption that traditional tribes that
have survived into the present provide a window to life 40,000 years ago is a
very big leap to make. But David Wengrow, Professor of Comparative Archaeology
at University College and David Graeber, Professor of Comparative Anthropology
at the London School of Economics, have together make a concerted attempt* to
imagine what life was like many thousands of years ago, rather than relying on
intuition or political preference.
Their
conclusion is that hunter-gatherer bands consciously switched from
hierarchical social arrangements at certain times of the year to resolutely
egalitarian forms at others. “Strongly dualistic patterns of organisation” as
the authors call them, existed, for example, in the glacial fringe of Europe
during the last Ice Age (which began to end around 20,000 years ago),
concurrent with tribes assembling in large numbers and then disassembling into
smaller groups. “The same population might experience entirely different
systems of economic relations, family structure and political life at different
times of year,” they say. Features like territoriality, social ranking or
material acquisitiveness would be put into effect at certain times of year and
then reversed.
Note, this is not just a matter – as in chimp bands – of one
alpha male seizing ‘power’ in summer, only for a rival to take the reins in
winter. Human bands, by contrast, knowingly switched the nature of social
arrangements.
“What makes us human,” say Wengrow/Graeber, “[is] the
inherent complexity of our political repertories, and in particular the range
of strategies for resisting domination, which far outstrip those available to
other primates. At the psychological level, these include ridicule, moral
censure and ostracism; at the social level they involve complex institutional
arrangements to limit or subvert the exercise of power.”
The reference to institutions is significant because power
in human societies is now mediated through institutions. In contrast to early
humanity, social arrangements are not easily reversed because they are fixed in
institutions which are intended to endure. These institutions control access to
resources and political power. Within them, shifting alliances, coups,
conspiracies, power grabs – ‘chimp-like’ behaviour if you will – may take
place. But their existence and effectiveness does not depend on the prevalence
of such behaviour. They rely on bare coercion and also obedience in non-privileged
ranks – ideologically-induced feelings that their power is justified and
morally correct.
'First' and 'Second' Nature
The social ecologist Murray Bookchin made an essential distinction
between consciously designed human social structures and animal communities,
the latter conditioned solely by instinct and idiosyncratic forms of behaviour.
In this sense, no animal group is hierarchical; the word – derived from the
ancient Greek meaning rule of the high priest – belongs firmly to the human
realm of institutional power. When we speak of hierarchies among animals, we
are merely projecting our own systems
of social ranking, whose contingency and non-biological origins we can’t deal
with, onto them. In his view:
… dominance and submission must be
viewed as institutionalized relationships,
relationships that living things literally institute or create but which are
nether ruthlessly fixed by instinct on the one had nor idiosyncratic on the
other. By this, I mean that they must comprise a clearly social structure of coercive and privileged ranks that exist quite
apart from the idiosyncratic individuals who seem to be dominant within a given
community, a hierarchy that is guided by a social logic that goes beyond
individual interactions or inborn patterns of behaviour. The Ecology of Freedom, p 94
Bookchin distinguished between ‘first’ and ‘second’, or
social, nature. First nature is the realm of human beings’ animality. We are
mammals, and primates and apes and have “natural, primal urges”. But at the
same time – and singularly among living things – we exhibit choice and
discretion about how we live together. Though human social forms might appear
static, a cursory glance at history reveals that they do change both
incrementally and explosively. Or as Noam Chomsky succinctly
expresses it, “humans are unique in the natural world in that they have
history, cultural diversity and cultural evolution”.
It is important to realise, however, that first and second
nature are not isolated, abstract categories. As Bookchin emphasised, in the
evolution of homo sapiens first nature gradually phased into second nature. Our
propensity to function cooperatively with each other is a product of first
nature, for example. “Quasi-biological institutions” around family, kinship,
age and gender – neither definitively part of first or second nature – still
play a major part in social life. Nationalism, racism and religious bigotry,
for example, can be compared to the proclivity for chimp bands to aggressively
attack strangers, in contrast to our awareness, however honoured in the breach,
of the existence of a universal humanity. There is always the danger, said
Bookchin**, of “our animality conspiring with our intelligence or cunning to
yield unforeseeable terrors and unexpected horrors”. But the element – perhaps
the burden – of choice is always present. This, at its most basic level, is
what freedom means.
Beware the Lobster
So human beings are both like other animals and not like
them. Perhaps this can be best illustrated by the aforementioned lobster. The
psychologist and “classical
liberal” Jordan Peterson, for example, claims that it’s inevitable “that
there will be continuity in the way animals and human beings organise their
structures”. The dominance hierarchy, he points out, is a “near eternal aspect
of the environment”. Extreme economic inequality among humans has the same
ultimate cause as the certainty that lobsters will fight over who has access to
the best hiding places – a perpetual striving for dominance and survival that
only a small minority can win. “It’s winner-take-all in the lobster world, just
as it is in human societies,” says Peterson,
“where the top 1 percent have as much loot as the bottom 50 percent – and where
the richest eighty-five people have as much as the bottom three and a half
billion.”
He is quite enamoured of the revelation that
anti-depressants can be successfully administered to lobsters – a defeated,
hunched lobster, low in serotonin, can be perked up and readied for battle
again by giving it Prozac. It will “advance on former victors and fight longer
and harder”. Despite the evolutionary gulf that separates them, humans and
lobsters share basic neuro-chemistry. “The drugs prescribed to depressed human
beings, which are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors,” says Peterson “have
much the same chemical and behavioural effect.”
But the fact that humans and lobsters share the same neurotransmitters
– and both can feel pain – does not mean we share the same social structures.
For one thing, lobsters don’t have a social structure. And for this reason,
anti-depressants, while they similarly affect personal behaviour in both
lobsters and humans, are less than useless when it comes to changing human
social structures. In contrast to the revived lobster, the mass prescribing of
anti-depressants that has taken place in the developed world since they were
first introduced in the late 1980s – by 2004 Prozac had been prescribed to 50
million people – has not made the slightest impact on social structures or made
those structures more permeable to people of low social rank. The same period
that has seen the wholesale dispensing of anti-depressants, undoubtedly one of
great business success stories of the last thirty years, has also witnessed sharp
declines in social mobility in the US and UK. Rather than opening up
careers and better incomes to people previously shut out from them, they have
instead simply relieved the pain induced by increasingly segregated and mapped
out lives.
Social classes and social strata are, in Bookchin’s
description, “made of sterner stuff” than individual behavioural traits. Changing
them, therefore, cannot be achieved by reforming individual behaviour. Ironically,
however, the belief that socially-created forms of domination and hierarchy
ultimately stem from the way humans behave – are biologically determined, in
other words – can shield them from change. To free ourselves, we first need to
stop believing.
*Wengrow & Graeber’s first paper on the subject – “Farewell
to the ‘Childhood of Man’: Ritual, Seasonality and the Origins of Inequality”
– was published in 2015. Another joint essay – “How to change the
course of human history” – was published in 2018 and touched on similar
themes. And I believe a book is forthcoming.
** Bookchin, who died in 2005, is a neglected thinker in my
opinion. His books include Re-enchanting
Humanity (where this quote is from), The
Ecology of Freedom and Remaking
Society.
No comments:
Post a Comment